What a transparent Romance language with a Germanic gender-determiner mapping tells us about gender retrievalInsights from European Portuguese

  1. Ana Rita Sá-Leite 1
  2. Ângela Tomaz 2
  3. Juan Andrés Hernández- Cabrera 3
  4. Universidad de Santiago de Compostela 1
  5. Carlos Acuña Fariña 1
  6. Montserrat Comesaña 4
  1. 1 Universidade de Santiago de Compostela
    info

    Universidade de Santiago de Compostela

    Santiago de Compostela, España

    ROR https://ror.org/030eybx10

  2. 2 Universidad de Lille
  3. 3 Universidad de La Laguna
    info

    Universidad de La Laguna

    San Cristobal de La Laguna, España

    ROR https://ror.org/01r9z8p25

  4. 4 Universidad Nebrija
    info

    Universidad Nebrija

    Madrid, España

    ROR https://ror.org/03tzyrt94

Revista:
Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

ISSN: 1576-8597

Ano de publicación: 2022

Volume: 43

Número: 2

Tipo: Artigo

DOI: 10.20350/DIGITALCSIC/14777 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso aberto editor

Outras publicacións en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental

Resumo

The study of the representation and processing of grammatical gender during language production has encountered mixed results regarding which conditions must be met to observe gender effects and whether these reflect the selection of gender values or competition between elements of agreement. The answer seems to depend on the number of determiners associated to each gender and on the language being explored. The present study aims to assess this issue through three picture-word interference tasks in European Portuguese. This is a transparent Romance language featuring a one-to-one gender-determiner mapping system similar to opaque Germanic languages. Conditions of gender in/congruency between targets and distractors were considered along with gender transparency and agreement. We observed a gender congruency effect restricted to noun phrases. Importantly, the effect was modulated by transparency, which seems relevant regardless of agreement. To explain the results, we adapted the Dual-Route Model of language comprehension to production.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • Adamson, L., & Šereikaitė, M. (2019) Representing gender and defaults: Evidence from Lithuanian. Glossa: A journal of General Linguistics, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.860
  • Alario, F. X., & Caramazza, A. (2002). The production of determiners: Evidence from French. Cognition, 82, 179–223. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00158-5
  • Ayora, P., Peressotti, F., Alario, F.-X., Mulatti, C., Pluchino, P., Job, R., & Dell’Acqua, R. (2011). What phonological facilitation tells about semantic interference: A dual-task study. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00057
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Barreña, A. (1997). Desarrollo diferenciado de sistemas gramaticales en un niño vascoespañol bilingüe, en W. R. Glass & A. T. Pérez-Leroux (Eds.), Contemporary Perspectives on the Acquisition of Spanish (pp. 55-74). Cascadilla Press & Pennsylvania State University.
  • Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Pizzamiglio, L., D’Amico, S., & Hernandez, A. (1995). Gender and lexical access in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(6), 847–847. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206800
  • Bates, D., Maechler, M, Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Bi, Y., Wei, T., Janssen, N., & Han, Z. (2009). The contribution of orthography to spoken word production: evidence from Mandarin Chinese. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(3), 555–560. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.3.555
  • Bordag, D., & Pechmann, T. (2008). Grammatical gender in speech production: Evidence from Czech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37(2), 69–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-007-9060-0
  • Boutonnet, B., Athanasopoulos, P., & Thierry, G. (2012). Unconscious effects of grammatical gender during object categorisation. Brain Research, 1479, 72–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.08.044
  • Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An analysis of transformations (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 26(2), 211–252. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1964.tb00553.x
  • Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., & Tanaka, M. (2008). Contributions of animacy to grammatical function assignment and word order during production. Lingua, 118(2), 172–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2007.02.003
  • Bürki, A., Sadat, J., Dubarry, A.-S., & Alario, F.-X. (2016). Sequential processing during noun phrase production. Cognition, 146, 90–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.002
  • Bürki, A., Besana, T., Degiorgi, G., Gilbert, R., & Alario, F. -X. (2020). Representation and selection of determiners with phonological variants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45(7), 1287–1315. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000643
  • Caffarra, S., & Barber, H. A. (2015). Does the ending matter? The role of gender-to-ending consistency in sentence reading. Brain Research, 1605, 83–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.018
  • Caffarra, S., Janssen, N., & Barber, H. A. (2014). Two sides of gender: ERP evidence for the presence of two routes during gender agreement processing. Neuropsychologia, 63, 124–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.016
  • Caffarra, S., Siyanova-Chanturia, A., Pesciarelli, F., Vespignani, F., & Cacciari, C. (2015). Is the noun ending a cue to grammatical gender processing? An ERP study on sentences in Italian. Psychophysiology, 52(8), 1019–1030. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12429
  • Caramazza, A. (1997). How Many Levels of Processing Are There in Lexical Access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(1), 177–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432997381664
  • Caramazza, A., & Miozzo, M. (1997). The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: Evidence from the “tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon. Cognition, 64(3), 309–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00031-0
  • Caramazza, A., Miozzo, M., Costa, A., Schiller, N., & Alario, F.-X. (2001). A Cross-linguistic Investigation of Determiner Production. In E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 209–226). MIT Press.
  • Casado, A., Palma, A., & Paolieri, D. (2021). The scope of grammatical gender in Spanish: Transference to the conceptual level. Acta Psychologica, 218, 103361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2021.103361
  • Casado, A., Palma, A., & Paolieri, D. (2018). The Influence of Sex Information on Gender Word Processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 47(3), 557–583. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9546-3
  • Corbett, G. G. (2013). Number of Genders. In M. S. Dryer & M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology (Available online at http://wals.info/chapter/30, Accessed on 2021-09-28).
  • Corbett, G. G. (2000). Number. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139164344
  • Corrêa, L. M. S., Augusto, M. R. A., & Castro, A. (2011). Agreement and markedness in the ascription of gender to novel animate nouns by children acquiring Portuguese. Journal of Portuguese Linguistics, 10(1), 121–142. https://doi.org/10.5334/jpl.103
  • Costa, A., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). The gender congruity effect: Evidence from Spanish and Catalan. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(4), 381–391. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909699386275
  • Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M., & Job, R. (2005). Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(1), 42–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.007
  • Dell, G. S. (1990). Effects of frequency and vocabulary type on phonological speech errors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 5(4), 313–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969008407066
  • Finocchiaro, C., Alario, F. X., Schiller, N. O., Costa, A., Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (2011). Gender congruency goes Europe: A cross-linguistic study of the gender congruency effect in Romance and Germanic languages. Rivista di Linguistica, 23(2), 161–198.
  • Fodor, I. (1959). The origin of grammatical gender. Lingua, 8. 186–214. https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(59)90020-8
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, 35(1), 116–124. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03195503
  • Foucart, A., Branigan, H. P., & Bard, E. G. (2010). Determiner selection in romance languages: Evidence from French. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(6), 1414–1421. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020432
  • Gollan, T. H., & Frost, R. (2001). Two routes to grammatical gender: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30(6), 627–651. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1014235223566
  • Guasch, M., Boada, R., Ferré, P., & Sánchez-Casas, R. (2013). NIM: A web-based Swiss army knife to select stimuli for psycholinguistic studies. Behavior Research Methods, 45(3), 765–771. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0296-8
  • Harris, J. (1991). The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22(1), 27–62.
  • Hartsuiker, R. J., & Kolk, H. H. J. (2001). Error monitoring in speech production: A computational test of the perceptual loop theory. Cognitive Psychology, 42(2), 113–157. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2000.0744
  • Heim, S., Eickhoff, S. B., Friederici, A. D., & Amunts, K. (2009). Left cytoarchitectonic area 44 supports selection in the mental lexicon during language production. Brain Structure and Function, 213(4-5), 441–456. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-009-0213-9
  • Heim, S., Friederici, A. D., Schiller, N. O., Rüschemeyer, S. A., & Amunts, K. (2009). The determiner congruency effect in language production investigated with functional MRI. Human brain mapping, 30(3), 928–940. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20556
  • Ibrahim, M. H. (1973). Grammatical Gender: Its Origin and Development. De Gruyter Mouton. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110905397
  • Jescheniak, J. D., Schriefers, H., & Lemhöfer, K. (2014). Selection of freestanding and bound gender-marking morphemes in speech production: A review. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(6), 684–694. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.654645
  • Kilarski, M. (2005). On grammatical gender as an arbitrary and redundant category. In D. A. Kibbee (Ed.), History of Linguistics 2005: Selected papers from the Tenth International Conference on the History of the Language Sciences (ICHOLS X), 1–5 September 2005, Urbana-Champaign, Illinois (pp. 24-36). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://doi.org/10.1075/sihols.112.03kil
  • Kuchenbrandt, I. (2008). Cross-linguistic influences in the acquisition of grammatical gender? Arbeiten zur Mehrsprachigkeit, 86, 1–28.
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  • La Heij, W., Mak, P., Sander, J., & Willeboorsde, E. (1998). The gender-congruency effect in picture word task. Psychological Research, 61(3), 209–219. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004260050026
  • Lenth, R. V. (2016). Least-Squares Means: The R Package lsmeans. Journal of Statistical Software, 69(1), 1–33. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v069.i01
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–75. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776
  • McGuire, P. K., Silbersweig, D. A., & Frith, C. D. (1996). Functional neuroanatomy of verbal self-monitoring. Brain, 119(Pt. 3), 907–917. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/119.3.907
  • Meyer, A. S. (1996). Lexical access in phrase and sentence production: results from picture–word interference experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, 35(4), 477–496. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.0026
  • Meyer, A. S., & Schriefers, H. (1991). Phonological facilitation in picture-word interference experiments: Effects of stimulus onset asynchrony and types of interfering stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(6), 1146–1160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.6.1146
  • Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1999). The selection of determiners in noun phrase production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25(4), 907–922. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00531-4
  • Miozzo, M., Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (2002). The absence of a gender congruency effect in romance languages: A matter of stimulus onset asynchrony? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(2), 388–391. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.2.388
  • New, J., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(42), 16598–16603. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703913104
  • O'Rourke, P. (2007). The gender congruency effect in bare noun production in Spanish. Coyote Papers: Working Papers in Linguistics, Linguistic Theory at the University of Arizona, 15, 66–89.
  • Paolieri, D., Lotto, L., Morales, L., Bajo, T., Cubelli, R., & Job, R. (2010). Grammatical gender processing in romance languages: Evidence from bare noun production in Italian and Spanish. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 335–347. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902916803
  • Paolieri, D., Lotto, L., Leoncini, D., Cubelli, R., & Job, R. (2011). Differential effects of grammatical gender and gender inflection in bare noun production. The British Psychological Society, 102(1), 19–36. https://doi.org/10.1342/000712610X496536
  • Pinheiro, J. C., & Bates D. M. (2000). Mixed-Effects Models in S and S-PLUS. Springer-Verlag.
  • Protopapas, A. (2007). Check Vocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4), 859–862. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03192979
  • Roelofs, A. (2004). Error biases in spoken word planning and monitoring by aphasic and nonaphasic speakers: comment on Rapp and Goldrick (2000). Psychological Review, 111(2), 573–570. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.561
  • Roelofs, A. (2008). Tracing attention and the activation flow of spoken word planning using eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 353–368. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.353
  • Roelofs, A., Meyer, A. S., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1998). A case for the lemma/lexeme distinction in models of speaking: Comment on Caramazza and Miozzo (1997). Cognition, 69(2), 219–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(98)00056-0
  • Roelofs, A., & Piai, V. (2015). Aspects of competition in word production: reply to Mahon and Navarrete. Cortex, 64, 420–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.10.016
  • Sá-Leite, A. R., Fraga, I., & Comesaña, M. (2019). Grammatical gender processing in bilinguals: An analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(4), 1148–1173. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01596-8
  • Sá-Leite, A. R., Haro, J., Comesaña, M., & Fraga, I. (2021). Of beavers and tables: The role of animacy in the processing of grammatical gender within a picture-word interference task. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.661175
  • Sá-Leite, A. R., Luna, K., Fraga, I., & Comesaña, M. (2020). The gender congruency effect across languages in bilinguals: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(4), 677–693. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01702-w
  • Schiller, N. O., & Caramazza, A. (2003). Grammatical feature selection in noun phrase production: Evidence from German and Dutch. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 169–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00508-9
  • Schiller, N. O., & Caramazza, A. (2006). Grammatical gender selection and the representation of morphemes: The production of Dutch diminutives. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21(7-8), 945–973. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960600824344
  • Schriefers, H. (1993). Syntactic processes in the production of noun phrases. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, memory, and Cognition, 19(4), 841–850. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.841
  • Schriefers, H., & Teruel, E. (2000). Grammatical gender in noun phrase production: The gender interference effect in German. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(6), 1368–1377. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.6.1368
  • Soares, A. P., Costa, A. S., Machado, J., Comesaña, M., & Oliveira, H. (2017). The Minho Word Pool: Norms for imageability, concreteness and subjective frequency for 3,800 Portuguese words. Behavior Research Methods, 49(3), 1065–1081. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-016-0767-4
  • Soares, A. P., Iriarte, A., Almeida, J. J., Simões, A., Costa, A., Machado, J., França, P., Comesaña, M., Rauber, A., Rato, A., & Perea, M. (2018). Procura-PALavras (P-PAL): A web-based interface for a new European Portuguese lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, 50(4), 1461–1481. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1058-z
  • Soares, A. P., Machado, J., Costa, A., Iriarte, Á., Simões, A., de Almeida, J. J., Comesaña, M., & Perea, M. (2015). On the advantages of word frequency and contextual diversity measures extracted from subtitles: The case of Portuguese. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(4), 680–696. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.964271
  • Starreveld, P., & La Heij, W. (2004). Phonological facilitation of grammatical gender retrieval. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(6), 677–711. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960444000061
  • Szekely, A., D’Amico, S., Devescovi, A., Federmeier, K., Herron, D., Iyer, G., Jacobsen, T., Arévalo, A., Vargha, A., & Bates, E. (2005). Timed action and object naming. Cortex, 41(1), 7–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70174-6
  • Szekely, A., Jacobsen, T., D’Amico, S., Devescovi, A., Andonova, E., Herron, D., Lu, C. C., Pechmann, T., Pléh, C., Wicha, N., Federmeier, K., Gerdjikova, I., Gutierrez, G., Hung, D., Hsu, J., Iyer, G., Kohnert, K., Mehotcheva, T., Orozco-Figueroa, A., Tzeng, A., ... & Bates, E. (2004). A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 247–250. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.002
  • Van Berkum, J. J. A. (1997). Syntactic processes in speech production: The retrieval of grammatical gender. Cognition, 64(2), 115–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00026-7
  • Vasishth, S. & Gelman, A. (2021). How to embrace variation and accept uncertainty in linguistic and psycholinguistic data analysis. Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/zcf8s
  • Velnić, M. (2020) Acquisition of a transparent gender system: A comparison of Italian and Croatian. Frontiers, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.571674
  • Vigliocco, G., Lauer, M., Damian, M., & Levelt, W. J. (2002). Semantic and syntactic forces in noun phrase production. Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 28(1), 46–58. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.28.1.46
  • Wilshire, C., Singh, S., & Tattersall, C. (2016). Serial order in word form retrieval: New insights from the auditory picture–word interference task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 299–305. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0882-8