Semiosis icónica en el desarrollo del lenguaje infantil

  1. González Pereira, Miguel
Revista:
Pragmalinguistica

ISSN: 1133-682X

Ano de publicación: 2020

Título do exemplar: Investigación en Lingüística Clínica

Número: 2

Páxinas: 179-198

Tipo: Artigo

DOI: 10.25267/PRAGMALINGUISTICA.2020.IEXTRA2.11 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openAcceso aberto editor

Outras publicacións en: Pragmalinguistica

Obxectivos de Desenvolvemento Sustentable

Resumo

Recent papers claim the relevance of iconicity in linguistic acquisition, but it is usually identified as a relationship of similarity between expression and content, thus referring to a simplifying view of Peirce's doctrine. The object of Peircean semiotics is not signs but semiosis as the only process of possible cognitive representation. It’s from this approach that he distinguishes three basic phenomenological categories, the first of which (Firstness) is linked to the iconic semiosis based on the qualities of the perceived. In this paper we propose a reading of Peirce’s semiotics that attends to his vision of iconism as the foundation of all semiosis and we review recent approaches to iconicity. We project this vision of iconicity to the analysis of the peculiarities of phonic processes and to the role of repetitions in child language through the exploitation of the Koinécorpus, raising its possible interest in clinical linguistics studies

Referencias bibliográficas

  • CP: PEIRCE, C. S. (1931-1935): The Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. Vols. I-VI, Hartshorne, Ch. & Weiss, P. (eds.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Vols. VII-VIII, Burks, A. W. (1958) (ed.), Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • ECO, U. (1975): Trattato di semiotica generale, Milano: Bompiani. Trad. cast. de C. Manzano, Tratado de semiótica general, Barcelona: Lumen, 1977.
  • ECO, U. (1992): La production des signes, Paris: Librairie Générale Française.
  • ECO, U. (1997): Kant e l’ornitorrinco, Milano: R.C.S. Libri. Trad. cast. de H. Lozano, Kant y el ornitorrinco, Barcelona: Lumen, 1999.
  • FERNÁNDEZ LÓPEZ, I. y PREGO, G. (2014): “Exploración lingüística del habla infantil”, Fernández Pérez, M. (coord.): Lingüística y déficit comunicativos, Madrid: Síntesis, pp. 45-99.
  • FERNÁNDEZ PÉREZ, M. (2014): “Roman Jakobson y su contribución al estudio del lenguaje peculiar”, Calero, Mª L. et al. (eds.): Métodos y resultados actuales en Historiografía de la Lingüística, Münster: Nodus Publikationen, pp. 193-203.
  • GALLESE, V. & LAKOFF, G. (2005): “The brain’s concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge”, Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22 (3/4), pp. 455–479.
  • GIVÓN, T. (1985): “Iconicity, isomorphism, and non-arbitrary coding in syntax”, Haiman, J. (ed.): Iconicity in Syntax, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 187-220.
  • GLENBERG, A. M. & GALLESE, V. (2012): “Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension and production”, Cortex, 48 (7), pp. 905-922.
  • GOMILA, T. & CALVO, P. (2008): “Directions for an Embodied Cognitive Science: Toward an Integrated Approach”, Gomila, T. & Calvo, P. (eds.): Handbook of Cognitive Science. An embodied approach, Oxford/Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, pp. 1-25.
  • GONZÁLEZ PEREIRA, M. (2018): “Aproximación semiótica al desarrollo del lenguaje infantil en edad temprana”, Díaz, M. et al. (eds.), Actas do XIII Congreso Internacional de Lingüística Xeral, Vigo: Universidade de Vigo, pp. 439-446.
  • HAIMAN, J. (1980): “The Iconicity of Grammar: Isomorphism and Motivation”, Language, 56 (3), pp. 515-540.
  • IMAI, M. & KITA S. (2014): “The sound symbolism bootstrapping hypothesis for language acquisition and language evolution”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369: 20130298.
  • JAKOBSON, R. (1941): Kindersprache, Aphasie und Allgemeine Lautgesetze, Uppsala: Universitets Arsskrift. Trad. cast. de E. Benítez (sobre versión francesa), Lenguaje infantil y afasia, Madrid: Ayuso.
  • KÖHLER, W. (1929): Gestalt Psychology. New York: Liveright.
  • LAING, C. et al. (2017): “How sa-lient are onomatopoeia in the early input? A prosodic analysis of infant-directed speech”, Journal of Child Language, 44, pp. 1117-1139. METEYARD, L. & VIGLIOCCO, G. (2008): “The Role of Sensory and Motor Information in Semantic Representation: A Review”, Gomila, T. & Calvo, P. (eds.): Handbook of Cognitive Science. An embodied approach, Oxford/Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, pp. 293-312
  • METEYARD et al. (2012): “Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics”, Cortex, 48, pp. 788-804.
  • METEYARD et al. (2015): “When semantics aids phonology: A processing advantage for iconic word forms in aphasia”, Neuropsychologia, 76, pp. 264-275.
  • OZTURK et al. (2013): “Sound symbolism in infancy: Evidence for sound-shape cross-modal correspondences in 4-months-olds”, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, pp. 173-186.
  • PERNISS, P. et al. (2010): “Iconi-city as a general property of language: evidence from spoken and signed languages”, Frontiers in Psychology 1, pp. 1-15.
  • PERNISS P. & VIGLIOCCO G. (2014): “The bridge of iconi- city: from a world of experience to the experience of language”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 369, 20140179.
  • PHARIES, D. (1985): Charles S. Peirce and the linguistic sign, Amsterdam: John Benjamins. PIAGET, J. (1959): La formation du symbole chez l’enfant. Neuchatel: Delachaux & Niestlé.
  • RICHARDSON, M. J. et al. (2008): “Ecological Psychology: Six Principles for an Embodied–Embedded Approach to Behavior”, Gomila, T. & Calvo, P. (eds.): Handbook of Cognitive Science. An embodied approach, Oxford/Amsterdam: Elsevier Science, pp. 161-187.
  • SAPIR, E. (1929): “A study in phonetic symbolism”, Journal of Experimental Psychology 12, pp. 225-239.
  • SLOBIN, D. (1985): “The child as a linguistic icon-maker”, Haiman, J. (ed.): Iconicity in Syntax, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 221-248.
  • SONESSON, G. (2008): “Prolegomena to a general theory of iconicity considerations on language, gesture, and pictures”, Klaas Willems, K. & De Cuypere, L. (eds.): Na- turalness and Iconicity in Language, Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 47-72.
  • SONESSON, G. (2013). “The natural history of branching: approaches to the phenomenology of firstness, secondness, and thirdness”, Signs and Society, 1 (2), pp. 297-326.
  • TOLAR, T. et al. (2008): “The development of the ability to recognize the meaning of iconic signs”, Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 13, pp. 225-240.
  • TOMASELLO, M. (2003): Constructing a language, Cambridge (MA): Harvard Univ. Press.
  • VAN LIER, L. (2004): The Ecology and Semiotics of Language Learning. A Sociocultural Perspective, Norwell: Kluwer Academic Publishers.