Comunicación referencial y conocimiento social¿independientes o conectados?

  1. Pérez Pereira, Miguel
  2. Resches, Mariela
Journal:
Anuario de psicología

ISSN: 0066-5126

Year of publication: 2004

Issue Title: La adquisición del lenguaje

Volume: 35

Issue: 2

Pages: 143-176

Type: Article

More publications in: Anuario de psicología

Bibliographic References

  • Astington, J. & Jenkins, J. (1995). Theory of mind development and social understanding. Cognition and Emotion, 9. 151-165.
  • Austin, J. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Trad. castellana: Cómo hacer cosas con palabras. Palabras y acciones. Barcelona: Paidós, 1982)
  • Austin, J. (1962). How to do things with words. Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Trad. castellana: Cómo hacer cosas con palabras. Palabras y acciones. Barcelona: Paidós, 1982)
  • Baldwin, D. & Moses, L. (2001). Links between early social understanding and word learning: challenges to current accounts. En A. Imbens-Bailey (Ed.), Special Issue of Social Development. 10, 309-29.
  • Baldwin, D. & Moses. L. J. (1994). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: evidence from language and emotion. En C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development (pp. 133-156). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum associates Ltd.
  • Baron Cohen, S. Leslie, A. & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a theory of mind? Cognition. 21, 37-46.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cohen, D. (Eds.) (1993). Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Cohen, D. (Eds.) (2000). Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neurosciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Bartsch, K. & Wellman, H. (1995). Children Talk about the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Bates, E. (1976) Language and Context: the acquisition of pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.
  • Benson, G., Abbeduto, L., Sort, K., Bibler Nuccio, J. & Maas, J. (1991). Perspective-taking skills of persons with mental retardation: Recognizing ignorance and false belief. Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Seattle, Washington, Abril.
  • Boada, H. & Forns, M. (1997a). The analysis of referential communication from an ecological approach. Anuario de Psicología. Monográfico: Perspectiva ecológica de la comunicación referencial.
  • Boada, H. & Forns, M. (1997b). Observational guidelines of communicative exchange: An ecological approach to referential communication. Anuario de Psicología. 75, 7-37.
  • Bonitatibus, G. (1988). What is said and what is meant in referential communication. En J. Astington, P. Harris & D. Olson (Eds. 1988), Developing Theories of Mind (pp. 326-338). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bretherton, I. (1991). Intentional communication and the development of an understanding of mind. En D. Frye & Ch. Moore (Eds.), Children's theories of mind: Mental states and social understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
  • Brown, J., Donelan-McCall, N. & Dunn, J. (1996). Why talk about mental states? The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings and mothers. Child Development, 67, 836-849.
  • Camaioni, L. & Ercolani, A. (1988). The role of comparison activity in the development of referential communication. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 403-413.
  • Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. (Eds.) (1996). Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  • Clark, H. H. & Wilkes-Gibbs, D. (1986). Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22, 1-39.
  • Conti-Ramsden, G. & Pérez-Pereira, M. (1999). Conversational interactions between mothers and their infants who are congenitally blind, have low vision, or are sighted. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 93, 691-703.
  • Cowan, P. (1967). The link between cognitive structure and social structure in two-child verbal interaction. Proceedings from the Annual Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development. Santa Mónica, 1967.
  • De Villiers (2000). Language and theory of mind: What are the developmental relationships? En S. Baron-Cohen; H. Tager-Flusberg & D. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neurosciences (pp. 83-123). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Dickson, W. (1982). Two decades of referential communication research: a review and meta-analysis. En C.J. Brainerd & M. Presley (Eds.), Verbal proceses in children (pp. 1-33). New York. Springer Verlag.
  • Dunn, J. & Brown, J. (1994). Affect expression in the family, children's understanding of emotion and their interaction with others. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 40, 120-137.
  • Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C. & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young children's understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development. 62, 1352-1366.
  • Flavell, J. H. (1988). The development of children's knowledge about the mind: From cognitive connections to mental representations. En J. W. Astington; P. L. Harris & D. Olson (Eds.), Developing Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Flavell, J. H., Botkin, P., Fry, C., Wright, J. & Jarvis, P. (1968). The development of role -taking and communication skills in children, New York: Wiley.
  • Flavell, J. H.; Speer, J., Green, F. & August, C. (1981). The development of comprehension monitoring and knowledge about communication. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46 (5) Serial # 192.
  • Franco, F. (1997). The development of meaning in infancy: early communication and social understanding. En S. Hala (Ed.), The development of social cognition (pp. 95-159). Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Garvey, C. (1984). Children's talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Traducción castellana: El habla infantil, Madrid, Morata, 1987).
  • Garvey, C. (1984). Children's talk. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Traducción castellana: El habla infantil, Madrid, Morata, 1987).
  • Glucksberg, S. & Krauss, R. (1967). What do people say after they learn to talk? Studies on the development of referencial communication. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 13, 309-316.
  • Grice, H. (1975) Logic and conversation. En P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics III: Speech acts (pp. 41-58). New York. Academic Press.
  • Gumperz, J. C. (1977). Situated instructions: Language socialization of school age children. En S. Ervin-Tripp & C. Mitchell-Kernan (Eds.), Child Discourse (pp. 103-121). New York: Academic Press.
  • Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101-119.
  • Harris, P. L. (1996). Desires, beliefs and language. En P. Carruthers & P. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 200-220). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Hedelin, L. & Hjelmquist, E. (1998). Preschool children's mastery of the form/content distinction in communicative tasks. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27 (3), 421-451.
  • Hogrefe, G., Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1986). Ignorance versus false belief: a developmental lag in the attribution of epistemic states. Child Development, 57, 567-582.
  • Hughes, C. & Dunn, J. (1997). «Pretend you didn't know»: Preschoolers' talk about mental states in pretend play. Cognitive Development, 12 (4), 381-403.
  • Jenkins, J. & Astington, J. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32, 70-78.
  • Lalonde, C. & Chandler, M. (1995). False belief understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional consequences of coming early or late to a first theory of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 167-185.
  • Leslie, A. & Frith, U. (1988). Autistic children representation of seeing, knowing and believing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 315-324.
  • Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Traducción castellana: Pragmática. Barcelona: Teide, 1989.)
  • Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Traducción castellana: Pragmática. Barcelona: Teide, 1989.)
  • Lewis, Ch. & Mitchell, P. (Eds.) (1994). Children's early understanding of mind: Origins and development. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Lloyd, P. (1990). Children's communication. En R. Grieve & M. Hughes (Eds.), Understanding Children: Essays in honor of Margaret Donaldson (pp. 51-70). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Lloyd, P., Baker, E. & Dunn, J. (1984). Children's awareness of communication. En L. Feagans; C. Garvey & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), The origins and growth of communication (pp. 281-296). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
  • Lloyd, P., Boada, H. & Forns, M. (1992). New directions in referential communication research. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 385-403.
  • Lohmann, H., Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74 (4): 1130-1144.
  • Mac Whinney, B, (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. 3 rd edition. Hillsdale, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Mc Tear, M. (1985). Children's conversations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
  • Menig-Peterson, C. L. (1975). The modification of communicative behavior in preschool-aged children as a function of the listener's perspective. Child Development, 46 (4), 1015-1018.
  • Miller, S. A. (2000). Children's understanding of preexisting differences in knowledge and belief. Developmental Review, 20, 227-282.
  • Mitchell, P. & Russel, J. (1991). Children's judgment of whether slightly and grossly discrepant objects were intended by a speaker. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 271-279.
  • Mitchell, P. & Russell, J. (1989). Young children's understanding of the say-mean distinction in referential speech. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 467-490.
  • Mitchell, P. (1996). Acquiring a conception of mind. Hove: Psychology press.
  • Ninio, A. & Snow, C. (1996). Pragmatic Development. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.
  • Núñez, M. (1993). Teoría de la Mente: Metarrepresentación, creencias falsas y engaño en el desarrollo de una psicología natural. Tesis Doctoral sin publicar. Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.
  • O'Neill, D, (1996). Two-year-old children's sensitivity to a parent's knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659-677.
  • Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (1979). Developmental Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.
  • Pechmann, T. & Deutsch, W. (1989). The development of verbal and non-verbal devices for reference. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 330-341.
  • Pérez-Pereira, M. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2001). The use of directives in verbal interactions between blind children and their mothers. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 95, 133-149.
  • Perner, J. & Davies, G. (1991). Understanding the mind as an active information processor: Do young children have a «copy theory of mind»? Cognition, 39, 51-69.
  • Perner, J. & Leekam, S. (1986). Believe and quantity: three year olds' adaptation to listeners knowledge. Journal of Child Language, 13, 305-315.
  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
  • Perner, J., Ruffman, T. & Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228-1238.
  • Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A. & Leekam, S. (1989). Exploration of the autistic child's theory of mind: Knowledge, belief and communication. Child Development, 60, 689-700.
  • Pillow, B. (1989). Early understanding of perception as a source of knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 116-129.
  • Resches, M. (2003). Desarrollo comunicativo y conocimiento social en niños preescolares. Tesis doctoral sin publicar. Universidad de Santiago de Compostela.
  • Rivière, A. y Núñez, M. (1996). La mirada mental. Buenos Aires: Aique.
  • Roberts, R. & Patterson, Ch. (1983). Perspective taking and referential communication: the question of correspondence reconsidered. Child development, 54, 1005-1014.
  • Rubin, K. (1978). Role-taking in childhood: Some methodological considerations. Child Development, 49, 428-433.
  • Sabbagh, M. & Baldwin, D. (2002). Understanding the role of perspective taking in young children's word learning. En N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. Mc Cormack & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint Attention: communication and other minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech Acts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Sodian, B. (1990). Understanding verbal communication: Children's ability to deliberately manipulate ambiguity in referential messages. Cognitive development, 5, 209-222.
  • Sonnenschein, S. & Whitehurst, G. (1984). Developing referential communication: a hierarchy of skills. Child Development, 55, 1936-1945.
  • Sonnenschein, S. (1988). The development of referential communication: Speaking to different listeners. Child Development, 59, 694-702.
  • Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
  • Taylor, M. (1988). Conceptual perspective taking: children's ability to distinguish what they know from what they see. Child Development, 59, 703-718.
  • Tomasello, M. (1999). Having intentions, understanding intentions and understanding communicative intentions. En J. Astington, D. Olson & Ph. Zelazo (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: social understanding and self-control (pp. 63-75). Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
  • Van Hekken, S., Vergeer, M. & Harris, P. (1980). Ambiguity of reference and listeners' reaction in a naturalistic setting. Journal of Child Language, 7, 555-563.
  • Welch-Ross, M. (1997). Mother-child participation in conversations about the past: Relationship with pre-schoolers' theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 33, 618-629.
  • Wellman, H. & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of Theory of Mind tasks. Child Development, 75 (2), 523-541.
  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). The Child's Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.
  • Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and the constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103-128.